Short Communication
Print
Short Communication
Observation of a cross-subfamily male-male mating attempt of grasshoppers in Kazakhstan (Orthoptera, Acrididae)
expand article infoOliver Hawlitschek, Nikita Sevastianov§, Jan-Henrik Pamin|, Martin Husemann
‡ Universität Zürich, Zürich, Switzerland
§ Institute for Information Transmission Problems of the Russian Academy of Sciences, Moscow, Russia
| Leibniz Institut zur Analyse des Biodiversitätswandels, Museum der Natur, Hamburg, Germany
¶ Staatliches Museum für Naturkunde Karlsruhe, Karlsruhe, Germany
Open Access

Abstract

Same-sex and interspecific sexual behavior have been documented in arthropods, but records remain very scarce. We report a mating attempt of a male Dociostaurus kraussi (Ingenitzky, 1897) (Gomphocerinae) and a male of Oedaleus decorus (Germar, 1825) (Oedipodinae), both grasshoppers (Orthoptera: Acrididae), from the region of the Dzungarian Alatau Mountains in Kazakhstan. A Disc3D scan showed that the cerci of the D. kraussi male clearly connect to the external genitalia of the O. decorus male, but we could not ascertain if the genitalia were inserted or if any sperm was transferred. We consider erroneous recognition the most likely explanation but discuss other potential reasons, concluding that much remains to be learned about the highly complex reproductive behavior of acridid grasshoppers.

Key Words

Dociostaurus, Dzungarian Alatau, interspecific sexual behavior, Kazakhstan, Oedaleus, same-sex sexual behavior

Introduction

Both same-sex sexual (or homosexual) behavior and interspecific sexual behavior have been documented in a variety of animal groups, including many arthropods (Bagemihl 1999). However, most of these records were the results of chance observations rather than of targeted scientific study. Consequently, it remains poorly understood if there is any adaptive benefit to these types of behavior, and if so, which (Scharf and Martin 2013). One potential explanation is that in environments of strong sexual male-male competition and imperfect sensory recognition mechanisms, mating with another male may be less costly than rejecting a female (Thornhill and Alcock 1983). This has been suggested, e.g., based on studies on Coleoptera and Diptera, some of which seem to rely on characters as simple as body size to distinguish females from males. Many species of Orthoptera, however, have evolved highly complex mate recognition systems in which both sexes employ a combination of acoustic, optical, and chemical signals to select conspecifics of the opposite sex during extensive courtship (Vedenina and Mugue 2011). While chemical signals may be misleading, e.g., in case of males carrying female pheromones from earlier matings, other types of signals can mostly be considered reliable. As a result, while hybrids of pairs of closely related species are known to occur in nature under certain conditions, mate choice has been found to be highly efficient (Gottsberger and Mayer 2007).

Methods

In the morning of June 26, 2023, we captured Orthoptera in a semi-desert locality (45.7957°N, 81.6379°E; ca. 655 m a.s.l) in the Northern foothills of the Dzungarian Alatau Mountains near Lake Alaqol, 10 km south of Koktuma, in the Jetisu Region of Kazakhstan. We transported around five to six live individuals per container in a total of seven containers for later processing at the camp. One container included, among others, a male of Dociostaurus kraussi (Ingenitzky, 1897) (Gomphocerinae; field number SOH_1561) and a male of Oedaleus decorus (Germar, 1825) (Oedipodinae; SOH_1560) (Fig. 1). Upon inspecting the container in the afternoon, we found that D. kraussi had mounted O. decorus and connected its cerci to the other male’s genitalia. This position persisted into the evening, and the individuals did not let go when removed from the container and when subsequently immersed into 80% ethanol for preservation. We did not detect any resistance behavior from the mounted O. decorus male. Later, we pinned and dried the mated couple and 3D imaged it using a Disc3D insect scanner at the Leibniz Institute for the Analysis of Biodiversity Change (LIB) Hamburg (Ströbel et al. 2018). The scan shows that the cerci of the D. kraussi male clearly connect to the external genitalia of the O. decorus male, but it remains unclear if the genitalia were actually inserted or if any sperm was transferred (Fig. 2, Suppl. material 1: fig. S1). The specimens are currently stored at the University of Zurich, Switzerland. We report due diligence in following the regulations of the Nagoya protocol, as per contacting the National Focal Point of Kazakhstan. No other research and export permits were required.

Figure 1. 

A. Male of Dociostaurus kraussi (Gomphocerinae; field number SOH_1561). B. Male of Oedaleus decorus. C. The sampling locality in the Jetisu Region of Kazakhstan. Photos: A, B. O. Hawlitschek, C. N. Sevastianov.

Figure 2. 

Disc3D scan screen capture of a male Dociostaurus kraussi (Gomphocerinae; field number SOH_1561) mounting and attempting to copulate with a male Oedaleus decorus (Oedipodinae; SOH_1560). See Suppl. material 1: fig. S1 for a complete movable scan. Scan: J.-H. Pamin.

Results and discussion

While no courtship behaviour has been demonstrated for O. decorus, D. kraussi are known to generate very simple songs. Like other Dociostaurini, D. kraussi uses a so-called wandering mating strategy (Helversen and Helversen 1994). Both sexes wander around their territories. Males periodically generate very simple calling songs (Savitsky 2000), but females are not known to produce any answers in this species. This suggests that males will try to copulate without waiting for an acoustic signal from females. From O. decorus, on the other hand, no acoustic communication is known at all. Under natural conditions, mating attempts between D. kraussi and O. decorus should still be extremely rare because of the chemical mating barriers present in most grasshopper species. It is therefore likely that the observed mating event is an artifact of captivity and possibly stress, where most mate recognition signals were inhibited. The copulation attempt of the D. kraussi male may have been stimulated by the strong similarity in body size of O. decorus males (18–31 mm) and D. kraussi females (19.3–31.5 mm). Nevertheless, the occurrence of this cross-subfamily male-male mating attempt and the fact that the mounting male remained mounted and with genitalia attached without letting go indicates that such events may occur in nature as well.

Under the conditions of captivity, we consider erroneous mate recognition the most likely explanation for the observed behavior. This, in turn, may be facilitated by the inclination of males to mount and attempt to mate in case of doubt due to lower cost, as described above. However, there may also be other potential benefits to the observed behavior. In their discussion on reproductive interference, Gröning and Hochkirch (2008) listed several cases in which insects used behavior that could reduce the reproductive success of conspecifics and other species. In Orthoptera, most documented cases consisted of signal jamming, i.e., individuals degrading the signals of other individuals (conspecifics or heterospecifics) by emitting signals of their own and thus complicating mate recognition. However, intraspecific male-male sexual behavior has been discussed as potential interference with the reproductive success of conspecific males. Between syntopic species competing for similar resources, interspecific same-sex mating behavior may interfere with the reproduction of the competitor and thus lower intraspecific competition for the offspring generation. To the male initiating this behavior, the cost is related to its inability to engage in sexual behavior that would lead to reproductive success at the same time. However, we suggest that the benefits might still outweigh the costs in case the male has already spent much of its sperm or if the density of females available for mating is low, in which case males could ‘block’ the males of a competing species while waiting for a conspecific mate. Such behavior may be beneficial to the individual of the species encountering a comparatively lower density of conspecific females, no matter whether it is the ‘active’ male initiating the mount or the ‘passive’ male allowing the mount.

Our observation is another example for the highly complex reproductive behavior of acridid grasshoppers. While many studies have already been conducted in this field, much certainly remains to be discovered.

Acknowledgements

We thank Varavara Vedenina for leading the expedition and Tatiana Tarasova for supporting the field work, Sebastian Schmelzle for technical support, the German Research Foundation for funding the position of J.H. Pamin, and the University of Hamburg for funding the Disc3D device.

References

  • Bagemihl B (1999) Biological exuberance: animal homosexuality and natural diversity. St. Martin, New York, 770 pp.
  • Gottsberger B, Mayer F (2007) Behavioral sterility of hybrid males in acoustically communicating grasshoppers (Acrididae, Gomphocerinae). Journal of Comparative Physiology A 193: 703–714. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-007-0225-y
  • Gröning J, Hochkirch A (2008) Reproductive interference between animal species. The Quarterly Review of Biology 83: 257–282. https://doi.org/10.1086/590510
  • Savitsky VY (2000) Acoustic signals, ecological features, and reproductive isolation of grasshoppers of the genus Dociostaurus (Orthoptera, Acrididae) in semi-desert. Entomological Review 80: 950–967.
  • Scharf I, Martin OY (2013) Same-sex sexual behavior in insects and arachnids: prevalence, causes, and consequences. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 67: 1719–1730. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-013-1610-x
  • Ströbel B, Schmelzle S, Blüthgen N, Heethoff M (2018) An automated device for the digitization and 3D modelling of insects, combining extended-depth-of-field and all-side multi-view imaging. Zookeys 759: 1–27. https://doi.org/10.3897/zookeys.759.24584
  • Vedenina V, Mugue N (2011) Speciation in gomphocerine grasshoppers: molecular phylogeny versus bioacoustics and courtship behavior. Journal of Orthoptera Research 20: 109–125. https://doi.org/10.1665/034.020.0111
  • von Helversen O, von Helversen D (1994) Forces driving coevolution of song and song recognition in grasshoppers. In: Schildberger K, Elsner N (Eds) Neural basis of behavioral adaptation. Gustav Fischer, Jena, 253–284.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material 1 

3D-scan of mating attempt of Dociostaurus kraussi and Oedaleus decorus

Author: Jan-Henrik Pamin

Data type: pdf

Explanation note: A male of Dociostaurus kraussi (Gomphocerinae) mounting a male of Oedaleus decorus (Oedipodinae) in a suspected mating attempt. The cerci of the D. kraussi male connect to the external genitalia of the O. decorus male. 3D image created with a Disc3D insect scanner at the LIB Hamburg. Open pdf in Adobe pdf viewer and click to rotate.

This dataset is made available under the Open Database License (http://opendatacommons.org/licenses/odbl/1.0/). The Open Database License (ODbL) is a license agreement intended to allow users to freely share, modify, and use this Dataset while maintaining this same freedom for others, provided that the original source and author(s) are credited.
Download file (117.76 MB)
login to comment